Block I Illinois Library Illinois Open Publishing Network

21 An Example: Building a Summary

In this chapter, we will go through one technique—a reasonably useful one—for building a careful critical summary of several sets of research findings on a single question: “How much violence is in popular media?” The question of violence in media is both important and trivial. Trivial, because the summary of research for any policy question—or any discussion of any research question, for that matter—would go through a similar process of incorporating results into a description of what is known. Important, because the question determines which research articles are relevant to include and which are not.

To mimic the actual process, for each article, we will start with the notes taken from the methods and the findings of one of the research papers. Generally speaking, the introduction, the review of literature, and the discussion are not included in the summary.

(Note: The introduction generally establishes why a particular research paper is a) a contribution to an important practical problem, b) an important theoretical or methodological problem, or c) an intrinsically interesting problem. Since the starting point of the summary is “How much violence do popular media show?” the reason why each study selected is relevant to the summary is already implicitly given. The review of literature is essentially hearsay evidence,[1] or what the author(s) of the article feels is important in setting up why they are doing the study. While I frequency use the literature review to look for additional papers to read, I generally go to the referred article itself before using any finding cited in a literature review. The discussion section is essentially where the researcher is reviewing the findings and/or extending the findings to other situations (either theoretical or practical). Since the nonrepetitive portion of the discussion section is essentially hypothetical, information from the discussion section is not valid as findings. Hence, the summary should be careful to summarize information about how the findings were developed (the methodology section) and what the findings are (the results or findings section).

Box 21.1 is detailed notes from “Trends of Sexual and Violent Content by Gender in Top-Grossing U.S. Films, 1950-2006,” a content analysis study of top grossing films (Bleakley, Jamieson, & Romer, 2012). The notes start with a check on the soundness of the study, move on to a discussion of study weaknesses, and then to the findings.

Box 21.1: Study 1 Overview

Study 1. Trends of Sexual and Violent Content by Gender in Top-Grossing U.S. Films, 1950-2006

Publication

Amy Bleakley, Patrick E. Jamieson, and Daniel Romer, “Trends of Sexual and Violent Content by Gender in Top-Grossing U.S. Films, 1950-2006,” Journal of Adolescent Health 51, no. 1 (July 2012): 73-79, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.02.006.

Method 

Population. U.S. films from 1950 to 2016

Sample. The top 30 movies each year from Variety’s annual list of top-selling 200 U.S. films (1950 to 2006). Researchers selected every other film in each year’s list (total 855 films).

Coders. Twenty-four trained undergraduates.

Code for violence. Violence was defined as “intentional acts (e.g., to cause harm, to coerce, or for fun) where the aggressor makes or attempts to make some physical contact that has potential to inflict injury or harm.”

Intercoder reliability. Krippendorff’s alpha .73 – .77

Weaknesses

Population. Does not cover changes in film content since 2006. Limited to movies shown to U.S. audiences. Biased toward top grossing films.

Coding categories. Emotional violence was not examined.

Findings

Violent content was high, present in 89 percent of all films.

Films that showed explicit sex and violence were significantly correlated.

Both genders started and received violence. Males started and received violence slightly more often than women.

The proportion of violence started and received increased for both genders over time.

Discussion of the Study

Bleakley et al. selected samples from Variety’s annual list of top-selling movies. Variety uses data compiled by Nielsen EDI and Rentrak Theatrical figures, two of the most reliable sources of data for television viewing and box office (movie) revenue. Variety is the premier source of entertainment industry news. Industry leaders closely watch it, which suggests that errors in the lists would be noticed and corrected.

Further, since the researchers sampled the most popular movies, they also captured the movies with the widest audience attention, which, arguably, have the most general impact. (Films that appeal to smaller niche audiences are—by definition—less likely to be sampled.)

Box 21.2: Summary of Findings, First Draft

A study of top-grossing movies from 1950-2006 found that 89 percent had violent content. Male characters were slightly more likely than women to deliver and to receive violent acts. Violent acts increased each decade after 1960 for both genders. Films that showed explicit sex were also more likely to show violence. (Beakley, Jamieson and Romer 2012.)

The first look at the research findings should be an answer to the main question of the research summary—in this case, is there violence in media? Looking specifically at Bleakley et al., the reader can say—with a fair degree of confidence—that yes, there is violence in top-grossing films and, in fact, most of the films studied were violent (89 percent). Including the words “top-grossing films” in the write-up also gives a fair clue to the careful reader on how the sampling limits findings (see Box 21.2).

The next step is to look for interesting details that show the scope of the violence, including the differences in other relevant factors, such as gender and time. (Both genders started and received violent acts, males slightly more often than women, and all violence increased over time.)

This particular study included other findings which were less relevant to the major question. Generally, only findings relevant to the major question of the summary are included.

Box 21.3: Study 2 Overview

Study 2. Violent Frames: Analyzing Internet Movie Database Reviewers’ Text Description of Media Violence and Gender Differences from 39 Years of U.S. Action, Thriller, Crime, and Adventure Movies

Publication

Jordy F. Gosselt, Joris J. Van Hoof, Bastiaan S. Gent, Jean-Paul Fox, “Violent Frames: Analyzing Internet Movie Database Reviewers’ Text Descriptions of Media Violence and Gender Differences from 39 Years of U.S. Action, Thriller, Crime, and Adventure Movies,” International Journal of Communication 9 (2015): 547-567, https://ijoc.org/index.php/ijoc/article/view/2921.

Method

Population. U.S. crime films

Sample. Synopses of crime movies published in IMDB from 1973 to 2011 using the genres action, thriller, crime and/or adventure. Movies with “unrealistic violence” were excluded—science fiction, horror, animation, sports (the violence is not the goal), westerns (the violence is historical), and war (the violence is politically endorsed).

Of the 8,932 movies within the realistic violence category, only 16 percent had more than three lines of text describing the movie, leaving a sample size of 1,396 usable synopses.

Coders. Two coders on 10 percent of the entire sample

Code for Violence. The codes for the types and severity of violence were based on data from the FBI Uniform Crime Reports (2009). The categories were as follows: Gun assault, blade assault, physical assault, projectile assault, rope assault, vehicular assault, chemical assault, environmental assault, explosive assault, forced drug use, sexual assault, and unknown assault. In addition to the type of violence, the researchers looked at severity—“light” (e.g., single hits and slaps), “severe” (bloody injury), “lethal,” or “unknown.”

Intercoder reliability. Cohen’s Kappa coefficient = .805

Weaknesses

Sample. One weakness is that the synopses are one step removed from the movie itself and may not accurately reflect the level of violence in the film. Further, writing synopses and assigning genres in developing IMDB content is a volunteer process similar to contributing to Wikipedia.[2] The quality and accuracy of the descriptions can vary widely from reviewer to reviewer, and the researcher does not know whose synopsis is accurate and whose is not. Also, not all movies were reviewed: the number of movies with synopses ranged from a low of 10 percent to a high of 40 percent of all movies released a given year. Again, the bias introduced is not known.

The researchers were primarily concerned with the level of realistic violence, i.e. the violence “that a viewer might actually encounter or read about in the news.” They classified action, thriller, crime, and adventure as realistic violence, and excluded genres with “unrealistic” violence, including science fiction, fantasy, horror, animation, sports, westerns, and war movies. The analysis then did not reflect the level of types of violence in a substantial subset of all movies.

Coding categories: Emotional violence was not examined.

Findings

Violence found

Firearms (39.5% | n = 1,335),

Physical violence (24.8% | n = 839),

Bladed weapons (8.4% | n = 283).

Other, including sexual assaults or violence with chemicals, vehicles, illicit drugs, ropes, and explosives, ranged from 4 to 124 counts (13.7% | n = 464).

Harm

Lethal harm. 54.9% (1,855 acts); Nonlethal: 41.1% (1,387 acts)

Lethal violence type:

Gun violence (67% | n = 898) were fatal shootings)

Blade assaults (58% were lethal| 165 out of 283)

Environmental violence (73% | 91 out of 124), chemical assaults (51% | 35 out of 69), explosive assaults (93% | 63 out of 68), vehicular violence (53% | 31 out of 59), projectile assaults (56% | 29 out of 52), and rope assaults (100% | all 39)

Physical violence (10% lethal l 10% (84 out of 839)

Gender and Violence

Male victims (79.2% | n = 2,676) and perpetrators (80.0% | n = 2,704)

Female victims and perpetrators (13.1% l n = 443 for both)

Male to male violence (63.1% | n = 2,132)

Female perpetrators to male victims (10.3% | n = 348)

Female to female violence (2.2% l n = 74)

Male victim probability of lethal violence decreases significantly over the study period (Wald = 4.838, p < .001)

Other

 Gun use does not vary from 1973 to 2011.

Introducing New Material into a Research Summary

Each additional research study should introduce new findings, which will either reinforce, expand, or limit the scope of what can be said about a particular topic. The next study’s research question, “What is the level of violence in crime films set in the current world (i.e., excluding historical, science fiction, war, sports, and horror movies)?” is both clearly relevant to the policy question, but from a more limited population. Therefore, we automatically know that the second study will likely underestimate violence in media by not including several genres of film known for having violent content (e.g., horror films, war films) as well as missing violence in other types of films (e.g., romance films) that are more likely to include emotional violence. (Note: the study method in Box 22.3 only tracks physical violence; emotional violence is not included.) Since errors run in one direction—that is, underestimating violence—the careful reader can say that the overall violence is probably more prevalent than what the study suggests.

The Gosselt et al. study (2015) sampled a different population (coding synopses of crime films) than the Bleakley et al. study (top grossing films) (2012), but similarly to Bleakley et al., the synopses of films also likely underestimated the actual acts of violence, given that synopses writers are more likely to overlook a violent act that is not central to the plotline than to introduce violence that did not appear in the film. Again, a literature summary should include enough information that the careful reader can determine bias introduced in the sampling (See Box 21.4). This information is contained within the initial description of the study sample “an analysis of synopses of present-time crime genre movies.” The researchers obviously will not tell the reader that the population they studied is not the population that the reader is interested in; readers will have to do that work on their own.

In terms of what findings to include, the literature review should include a description of the level of violence found (the main answer to the literature review question) and additional specific information relevant to the main question. Any of the following would qualify as fleshing out the main question:

Gun violence was, by far, the most common type of violence (54 percent) and guns the most lethal (67 percent were fatal shootings).

Both men and women were aggressors and victims, but men were far more likely to be the victim (79 percent) and the aggressor (80 percent). Women were aggressors or victims 13.1 percent of the time for each category.

More recent films were less likely (slightly) to have nonlethal violence.

A rewritten literature review (see Box 21.4) combining both studies should first state the new basic answer to the guiding question, and then point out interesting supporting details from both studies. (The words retained from the description of the study in Box 21.2 are in black, and additional information is in green.)

Box 21.4: Summary of Findings, Second Draft

How to Read: Original text is in black. Additions from the 2nd study are in blue and italicized. [Transcriber’s note: source text is referenced in brackets after text]

Major films consistently show a relatively high level of violence both in scope (89% of all top grossing action films from 1950 to 2011 (Bleakley et al 2012) [study 1] and in number of violent acts overall (Gosselt et al., 2015). [study 2] Men were more likely to deliver and receive violence, and in one study far more likely. Eighty percent of all violence in crime films were male on male (Bleakley et al 2012). The findings were mixed on whether violence increased over time with one study reporting increasing violence for both genders (Bleakley et al., 2012) [study 1] and another indicating that male deaths from gun violence decreased significantly over time (Gosselt 2015). [study 2]

The findings reported should support or qualify the major claim of the topic sentence and not wander off onto other topics. The exact findings used for supporting details will probably vary from writer to writer. For example, Box 21.3 included evidence that supports the claim that movies show a consistently high level of violence, but did not discuss guns or blades versus physical contact (hands, feet etc.). That information would have been fine to include because it helped characterize the violence, but not absolutely necessary. Including that information, however, would be absolutely necessary if the main question for the policy analyst was, “What is the level gun violence in media?”

Looking broadly at the main question of violence in media, the two studies discussed so far only cover one medium (movies) and one particular type of violence (physical). As such, the studies’ findings are an incomplete answer. The two additional studies broaden the scope to include other types of media (music videos and TV reality shows) and other types of violence (emotional/psychological violence).

Box 21.5: Overview of Study 3

Study 3. Violence in Music Videos: Examining the Prevalence and Context of Physical Aggression

Publication

Stacy L. Smith and Aaron R. Boyson, “Violence in Music Videos: Examining the Prevalence and Context of Physical Aggression,” Journal of Communication 52, no. 1 (January 2002): 61-83, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-2466.2002.tb02533.x.

Method

Sample. A composite week of music video programming across 20 weeks. (Data gathered during the 1996-1997 television season.) The researchers randomly sampled three popular (at the time of the study) music video channels: Black Entertainment Television, Music Video Television, and Video Hits-One.

Coders. Fifty-six trained undergraduates

Code for violence: Same coding scheme (as above) for violent acts

Intercoder reliability. The intercoder reliability ranged from .67 to 1.0. The type of intercoder reliability was not included.

Coding variables below .7 are:

Pattern of punishment for bad characters = .67

Pattern of punishment for good characters = .68

Additional coding approaching low reliability:

Pain = .70

Depicted harm = .76

Likely harm = .77

Weaknesses

A significant problem with the sample (in terms of applying the findings to a current situation) is that the study was conducted using data from 1997 (over twenty years old) and most of the shows sampled have substantially changed format since the sample was drawn. MTV has drastically changed its programming, BET has transferred its music programming to branded sister networks, and VH1 primarily shows reality television shows. Furthermore, music videos are now most commonly distributed over the web (e.g., YouTube), which has different content restrictions than television.

Coding categories. Emotional violence was not examined.

Intercoder reliability. The intercoder reliability is too low for two of the variables. These are “pattern of punishment for bad characters” (.67) and “pattern of punishment for good characters” (.68).

Findings

Across three channels, 15 percent of all videos in a given week had one or more acts of physical aggression. Of these, 80 percent had one violent interaction, 17 percent had two, and 3 percent had three or more violent acts.

The aggressor was most likely to be an adult (96 percent), male (78 percent), and Black (56 percent). The victims were also most likely to be adult, male, and Black. Males were more likely to be targets of violence in rap (84 percent) or rock (89 percent). The violence differed significantly by music type, with rap videos at 29 percent, rock at 12 percent, R&B at 9 percent, adult contemporary at 7 percent, and other at 9 percent. Just under one-third (32 percent) of all the violent interactions in music videos involved lethal violence that would result in serious physical harm in the real world.

Introduction of a Third Study into a Summary of Research Literature on Violence in Media

Box 21.6: Summary of Findings, Third Draft

How to Read: Original text is in black, additions from the 2nd study are in blue and italicized, and additions from the 3rd study are in red and underlined. [Transcriber’s note: source text is referenced in brackets after text]

Research [study 3] consistently found violence in media, although the amount varied widely from a low of 15% in music videos (Smith and Boyson 1997) to a high of [study 3] 89% of all realistic crime films (Gosselt 2015). [study 1] Major films consistently show a relatively high level of violence both in scope (89% of all top grossing action films from 1950 to 2011, Gosselt et al., 2015) and in number of violent acts overall (synopses of present-time crime genre movies from 1993-2011 described an average of 2.4 violent acts per film, Bleakley et al., 2012). [study 2] All studies showed that men were more likely to deliver and receive violence [study 1], and in one study far more likely (Bleakley et al., 2012).  Eighty percent of all violence in crime films was male on male violence. [study 2]

The studies of films [study 1 and 2] were mixed on whether violence is increasing over time, with one study reporting increasing violence for both genders (Bleakley et al., 2012), and another indicating that male deaths from gun violence were decreasing significantly over time (Gosselt 2015). [study 2] A study of music videos sampled in 1997 from three channels that showed popular music videos (Smith and Boyson 2002) also showed that both victims and perpetrators were disproportionately likely to be adult and male. The videos had fewer acts of violence and were less likely to show fatal events. Just under a third (32%) of the videos had lethal violence, defined as violence that would result in serious physical harm in the real world. [study 3]

The difficulty of adding the findings for the study on media violence, is how to handle questionable findings.  For the first time, the study is both old and problematic. First, the authors do not discuss how television channels select the music videos they show, so the direction of any sampling bias on the part of the TV producers is unclear. Second, the data was collected in 1997. There is no substantive reason to believe that music videos are the same now as they were then. Each of the television channels listed in the methods section of the research paper, “Violence in Music Videos,” have substantively changed their programming, and most music videos are on distribution channels (e.g., YouTube) that have far weaker content restrictions than mainstream television. So, what to do? Should you throw the data out because it is flawed? Well, no. It is not flawed enough that the research produced has no value. The study data shows that in 1997, music video violence on three channels (that were the major music video channels at the time the research was done) was relatively low. This is a valid finding for over twenty years ago—even though it does not demonstrate the incidence of violence in current music videos. A good literature review must include all sound relevant research studies, whether the studies agree with the majority of the rest of the findings or not.

Turning to the specific findings, the music video findings significantly weaken the claims that can be made about violence in media. Instead of saying that there is a uniformly “high level of violence,” the overall assessment will need to be softened to accurately reflect the additional knowledge (see Box 21.6, in red) that one study showed some (but not much) violence. The writer also included the year of the study, so that a careful reader will know how dated the research is.

Introduction of a Fourth Study into a Summary of Research Literature on Violence in Media

Box 21.7: Study 4 Overview

Study 4. Surviving Survivor: A Content Analysis of Antisocial Behavior and its Context in a Popular Reality Television Show

Publication

Christopher Wilson, Tom Robinson and Mark Callister, “Surviving Survivor: A Content Analysis of Antisocial Behavior and Its Context in a Popular Reality Television Show,” Mass Communication and Society 15, no. (February 2012): 261-283, https://doi.org/10.1080/15205436.2011.567346.

Method

Sample. Seven seasons of Survivor (92 episodes). Reunion shows and show teasers were excluded.

Coders. Two trained researchers

Code for Violence

Antisocial acts consisted of four mutually exclusive categories:

Theft: Taking another person’s property without that person’s consent or knowledge.

Verbal aggression: Any nonphysical act that places another person under duress with the intention to pressure, constrain, or persuade in a noxious manner, or any hostile remark meant to diminish another’s self-image or cause psychological harm.

Minor aggression: Physical aggression resulting with minimal harm or no harm (e.g., slapping, punching, and kicking).

Deceit: Misleading for personal gain (e.g., fraud, cheating, and lying).

Intercoder reliability. Krippendorff’s alpha = .85.

Weakness

Limited sample: One specific type of television show (reality), and one particular show within that category (Survivor).

Findings

There were 4,207 antisocial acts at a rate of 45.7 acts per hour. Indirect aggression and verbal aggression were the most frequently occurring types of antisocial behavior.

The Wilson, Robinson, and Callister (2012) study on the television reality show, Survivor, extended the definition of violence to include emotional aggression. This study examined multiple episodes of a single television reality show and found multiple instances of verbal, deceit, and indirect aggression (defined as acts that take “place behind the victim’s back”) (Wilson, Robinson, and Callister 2012).

The addition of this study means that the topic sentence would need to include both physical and emotional violence, and specific findings about the prevalence and kind of antisocial violence would need to be added in the evidence section (see Box 21.8; the additional changes to the literature review are in brown).

Box 21.8: Complete Literature Review

How to Read: Original text is in black, additions from the 2nd study are in blue and italicized, additions from the 3rd study are in red and underlined, additions from the 4th study are bolded and highlighted in yellow, and summary conclusions are italicized and underlined and highlighted in gray. [Transcriber’s note: source text is referenced in brackets after text]

Research consistently found violence in all media studied [study 4]music (15% of all music videos, Smith and Boyson 1997), [study 3] film (89%, Gosselt 2015 [study 2] and Bleakley et al., 2012), [study 1] and all reality shows (100%, Wilson, Robinson, and Callister 2012). [study 4] With the exception of a 20-year-old music video study, [study 3] all other studies showed high amounts of violence (between 54% and 100%) in television and film. [study 4] The most violence reported was from a 2015 study of all top grossing action films from 1950 to 2011, (Bleakley et al’s 2012 study, [study 1] a finding supported by Gosselt et al.’s study of synopses of present-time crime genre movies from 1993-2011 which described an average of 2.4 violent acts per film (Gosselt et al., 2015). [study 2]

Most studies showed that violence was increasing over time for both men and women. [study 4] The outlier for overall violence, music videos, still showed violence in just under a third of the videos. [study 3] Studies that covered multiple years all showed increases in violence over time (Bleakley et al 2012; Gosselt 2011, Wilson et al 2012), [study 4] although, one reported significant reduction in one category – male death from gun-related violence (Gosselt 2015). [study 2]

Men were involved in the vast majority of physical and a slight majority of emotional violence. [study 4] Eighty percent of all crime film violence was male on male (Bleakley et al 2012). [study 1] Both victims and perpetrators in music videos were disproportionately likely to be adult and male (Smith and Boyson 1997). [study 3] However, men were only slightly more likely than women to indulge in emotional violence (males = 54% of 3605 incidents, Wilson et al. 2012). [study 4]

The overall findings suggest a persistent pattern of violence (all studies), with high levels of lethal violence (film and music videos) increasing over time (film and reality shows).  The studies, however, do not cover digital distribution modalities – YouTube, Instagram, Snapchat – channels that have distinct distribution patterns and generally fewer restrictions suggesting that the actual level of mediated violence could be more extreme. [summary conclusions]

Finally, the writer should end the summary with any overall assessment that the writer feels is necessary to highlight for the reader, including important limitations of the research findings. For example, the research studies examined do not include all media. In particular, they do not cover digital media—YouTube, Instagram, or Snapchat. This means that the studies have ignored important distribution channels that systematically have fewer restrictions than traditional media. Since it is probable that the content of less regulated media is more violent, the lack of digital media studies is an important limitation that would bias the overall results. Overall, the literature review should give the reader an accurate sense of the answer to the major question, “Research consistently found violence in all media studied,” and enough evidence that the reader would agree with the topic sentence based on the evidence alone (Box 21.8) without actually reading the topic sentence. There is no specific, hard-and-fast rule as to which exact topic sentence the writer can choose to develop and what evidence the writer chooses to include, but there are some shared guidelines for what is acceptable in the topic sentence and what is not. No one, that is, no one, who is a rational reader can finish all four studies and say that the media showed no violent content. A writer who suggests no violent content would be delusional or lying. A writer who suggests that movies only showed limited violence would be misrepresenting the level and seriousness of lethal violence in the movies. Anyone in this culture who does not accept that lethal violence is severe is fundamentally detached from shared social reality.

Writers, however, have some choice on secondary research themes, such as:

The interaction between violence and gender (covered in most studies),

Whether or not the violence shown has consequences (an important factor in how likely viewers will model behavior they see in media and covered in some studies), and

The interactions between violence and race (an important issue in social justice and covered in most studies).

Any or all of these themes would be acceptable subcomponents of the larger theme of violence in media.

The research-by-research rewrite of a research summary is not necessarily the only method to use to construct a summary, given that people have different writing strategies. However, all summaries should have the following structure and inclusions:

    1. For each sound research paper, the findings of the paper relevant to the main question should be presented.
    2. The literature summary should start with a topic sentence that answers your main question: What did the research papers find?
    3. The rest of the paragraph should include pertinent details from individual research papers. The summary should include enough pertinent details that the reader would be able to develop a topic sentence strikingly similar to the actual topic sentence just from the details alone (without reading the topic sentence).
    4. The summary should include shadings and findings that qualify and/or contradict the main summary sentence (topic sentence) of a summary paragraph or paragraphs.
    5. The careful reader should be able to pick out where the population in each study differs from the population of interest in the summary. (This information, while important, is the most likely to be missed in extant summaries.)
    6. The paragraph(s) should end with a description of where the evidence is incomplete or missing.

  1. That is, information in the literature review is the author’s summary of what other researchers found, not what the researchers said in their own words. It is always best to directly go to the original research paper.
  2. “Plots,” IMDb Help Center, archived December 22, 2018, https://web.archive.org/web/20181222181356/https://help.imdb.com/article/contribution/titles/plots/G56STCKTK7ESG7CP?ref_=helpms_helpart_inline#.

License

Icon for the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License

Reading Social Science Methods Copyright © 2023 by Ann Reisner is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial 4.0 International License, except where otherwise noted.

Share This Book